LETTER TO THE EDITOR

The recognition of dental anxiety. A comment on ‘Dental clinicians recognizing signs of dental anxiety: a grounded theory study’ by M. Höglund, I. Wårdh, S. Shahnavaz and C. Berterö

 

Citation: ACTA ODONTOLOGICA SCANDINAVICA 2024; VOL. 83: 69. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/00016357.2023.2277252.

Copyright: © 2024 The Author(s). Published by MJS Publishing on behalf of Acta Odontologica Scandinavica Society. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), allowing third parties to copy and redistribute the material in any medium or format and to remix, transform, and build upon the material, with the condition of proper attribution to the original work.

Received: 7 July 2023; Revised: 23 October 2023; Accepted: 25 October 2023; Published: 26 March 2024.

Competing interests and funding: No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

 

We read the article, ‘Dental clinicians recognizing signs of dental anxiety: a grounded theory study’ by Höglund and colleagues [1] with interest and would like to offer some observations.

We welcome the research approach taken as it has been helpful in eliciting and systemising the hypotheses. This has enabled the development of a helpful construct – the clinical eye – which calibrates the process by which dental clinicians describe and categorize some elements of their identification of dental anxiety in real-life clinical practice. The authors comment that the clinical eye is ‘shorthand’ and omits some important aspects and defining features of a more complete understanding of the nature of dental anxiety.

The clinicians in the study used signs of sympathetic activation as an objective proof of dental anxiety. The authors rightly highlight that individuals may not display external signs of anxiety for a variety of reasons [24]. However, another significant reason is that the three major aspects of fear (behavior or avoidance, reported or cognitive fear, and physiological arousal) may be desynchronous or variably dominant in the individual’s presentation as fearful and, importantly, they may change at different rates [5,6]. It is our experience that cognitive fears are usually the last to change; one can be treating a completely co-operative, apparently relaxed patient, who suddenly uses the stop signal and then careful inquiry is necessary to elucidate the fear.

Therefore, we suggest that the ‘clinical ear’ is also crucial for identifying and responding appropriately to dental anxiety: i.e. clinicians must harness active listening skills to elicit and fully engage with expressions of cognitive fear - attending compassionately to verbal (in addition to non-verbal) cues from patients. These fears may be viewed by clinicians as ‘unrealistic’ (p. 5) but are very real to the patient. It is important that patients who report significant fears are believed, and it is unfortunate that some clinicians chose to disbelieve the patients unless they were showing signs of physiological arousal or cooperation difficulties (p. 7); this is classic desynchrony and the authors are correct when they state that this may lead to distrust of the dentist.

ORCID

Helen R. Chapman symbol http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7699-9832

Nima Moghaddam symbol http://orcid.org/0000-0002-8657-4341

References

[1]     Höglund M, Wårdh I, Shahnavaz S, et al. Dental clinicians recognizing signs of dental anxiety: a grounded theory study. Acta Odontol Scand. 2023;81(5):340–348. doi:10.1080/00016357.2022.2154263.

[2]     Lenk M, Berth H, Joraschky P, et al. Fear of dental treatment-an under-recognized symptom in people with impaired mental health. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2013;110(31–32):517–522. doi:10.3238/arztebl.2013.0517.

[3]     Ayer WA, Jr., Domoto PK, Gale EN, et al. Overcoming dental fear: strategies for its prevention and management. J Am Dent Assoc. 1983;107(1):18–27. doi:10.14219/jada.archive.1983.0184.

[4]     Daskalakis NP, Lehrner A, Yehuda R. Endocrine aspects of post-traumatic stress disorder and implications for diagnosis and treatment. Endocrinol Metab Clin North Am. 2013;42(3):503–513. doi:10.1016/j.ecl.2013.05.004.

[5]     Hodgson R, Rachman S. II. Desynchrony in measures of fear. Behav Res Ther. 1974;12(4):319–326. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(74)90006-0.

[6]     Rachman S, Hodgson R. I. Synchrony and desynchrony in fear and avoidance. Behav Res Ther. 1974;12(4):311–318. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(74)90005-9.

Helen R. Chapman symbol
School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Brayford Pool,
Lincoln, LN6 7TS, UK
hchapman@lincoln.ac.uk

Nima Moghaddam symbol
School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Brayford Pool,
Lincoln, LN6 7TS, UK

Nick Kirby-Turner
Clinical Psychologist, West Sussex, UK