SHORT COMMUNICATION

CHANGES IN FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY FOLLOWING INTENSIVE ATTENTION TRAINING IN PATIENTS WITH TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY. A PILOT STUDY

Hanna PERSSON, MSc1, Tie-Qiang LI, Adj. Prof2,3 and Gabriela MARKOVIC, PhD1,4

From the 1Division of Rehabilitation Medicine, Danderyd University Hospital, 2Department of Clinical Science, Intervention, and Technology, Karolinska Institutet, 3Department of Medical Radiation and Nuclear Medicine, Karolinska University Hospital, and 4Department of Clinical Sciences, Karolinska Institutet, Danderyd University Hospital, Stockholm, Sweden

Objective: To explore functional connectivity after intensive attention training in the chronic phase after traumatic brain injury as clinical evidence indicates that intensive attention training improves attention dysfunction in persons with traumatic brain injury.

Design and subjects: A case series study. Two young adults, 13- and 18-months post traumatic brain injury, with traumatic brain injury induced attention deficits were assigned to 20 h of intensive attention training and neuroimaging.

Methods: Functional magnetic resonance imaging during a psychomotor vigilance test was conducted pre- and post-intervention.

Results: The neuroimaging indicated both increased and decreased connectivity density in frontal, posterior and subcortical brain regions, for some regions with separate change patterns for left and right hemisphere respectively, and an overall reduction in variability in functional connectivity.

Conclusion: The changed and decreased variability of functional connectivity in various brain regions, captured by fMRI during a psychomotor vigilance test after direct attention training in a small sample of persons with traumatic brain injury, suggests further studies of functional connectivity changes in neural networks.

LAY ABSTRACT

In our study, we wanted to know how the brain changes after intensive attention training. Two young adults with traumatic brain injury (car accident and fall from height) were administered intensive attention training for 20 h. To measure the intervention effect, the patients underwent a brain scan in an MRI-camera, while performing a test of vigilance. The vigilance test requires that you use your attention. The brain imaging showed that the brain activity was more stable and with less variability after the attention training. These findings suggest the potential benefit of attention training in regulating neural networks. More studies are needed to better illustrate the linkage between changes in the brain and intervention effect.

Key words: attention process training; traumatic brain injury; fMRI; functional connectivity metrics.

 

Citation: JRM-CC 2024; 7: jrmcc12436. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2340/jrmcc.v7.12436

Copyright: © Published by Medical Journals Sweden, on behalf of the Foundation for Rehabilitation Information. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/)

Submitted: May 4, 2023; Accepted: Dec, 21, 2023; Published: Jan 16, 2024

Correspondence address: Gabriela Markovic, Department of Clinical Sciences, Karolinska Institutet, Danderyd University Hospital, Stockholm, Sweden. E-mail: gabriela.markovic@ki.se

 

Attention deficit is common after traumatic brain injury (TBI). Since attention supports other cognitive functions, remediation of impaired attention should have a high priority early in the rehabilitation process (1). To better understand the recovery process, studies concerning brain mechanisms underlying different remediation techniques are also highlighted (2). A series of neuroimaging studies has shown that different aspects of attention including monitoring skills are dependent on functional connectivity networks of the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and parietal cortex (38).

Attention training in patients with TBI has resulted in reduced frontoparietal activity suggesting a reduced demand for attentional processes post-training (9). Attention Process Training-II (APT-II) (1013) is recommended as a standard practice for attention dysfunctions post TBI (1416). The APT-II is a direct, process-based attention training (10), promoting awareness and generalization through metacognitive strategy training. It is psychometrically validated (1, 1720) with emerging radiological evidence suggesting reorganization of attention networks post training (21). Our objective is to explore functional connectivity after intensive attention training in the chronic phase after TBI exploring the potential impact of attention training on the frontoparietal attention network.

METHOD

The current pilot study is exploratory with a pre- and post-design of APT-II in 2 patients with TBI in the chronic phase. Twenty hours of APT-II was administered (1-h sessions, 3–5 times weekly). The inclusion criteria derive from a larger study (20): 18–40 years, > 12 months post TBI, > 7 scaled score (SS) in Matrices (logical reasoning) in Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV) (22), mild to moderate attention deficit as indicated by the APT-test (indicative test for severity and nature of attention deficit within the APT-II) defined as < 70% accuracy on 2 out of 5 sublevels of attention in the APT-test (10). Exclusion criteria: signs of dementia (history or CT-scan) (22); contraindications for functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Written information about the study was delivered and consent was obtained. The Glasgow Outcome Scale – Extended (GOSE) (23) was used to evaluate functional outcome and overall recovery. Pre- and post fMRI including inclusion assessment was conducted within a week adjacent to the treatment period.

Neuroimaging protocol

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data were acquired in a whole-body 3T clinical MRI scanner (Magnetom Prisma, Siemens Medical Solutions) equipped with a 64-channel phased-array receiving head coil. The protocol included: (i) Conventional clinical 3D MRI scans of T1-weighted MPRAGE, T2 and FLAIR; (ii) Diffusion tensor imaging of the whole brain; (iii) Perfusion mapping with PCASL; (iv) The longer session of 20 min of BOLD fMRI during a continuous psychomotor vigilance task (PVT) that requires sustained attention (24, 25) was used to avoid a learning effect in PVT (26). The main acquisition parameters for the BOLD fMRI included: TE/TR = 33.1/720 ms, flip angle = 52°, 72 slices of 2 mm thick, FOV = 208×180 mm, matrix size = 104×90. The data acquisition was accelerated twice with in-plane GRAPPA parallel imaging and further accelerated with a multiple band factor of 8 in the slice direction. A total of 1655 dynamic timeframes were acquired.

Functional connectivity (FC) metrics from the BOLD fMRI during the PVT reflect statistical associations between brain activity time series, like those derived from resting-state fMRI data (27). To remove first-order effect of task-evoked activations that can inflate FC estimates, we used a standard approach that flexibly fits and removes task-evoked responses. The fMRI datasets underwent a preprocessing pipeline (28, 29) based on programs from the AFNI software package (http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/afni), followed by a quantitative data-driven analysis framework (QDA) (30, 31). A sliding window approach was used to estimate FC between brain regions within shorter time segments. For each time segment, we used the QDA framework to compute the local (a given voxel in the brain) functional connectivity strength index (CSI) and connectivity density index (CDI) with the rest of the brain. The window length affects the temporal resolution of the analysis, and different window lengths are typically used to capture changes over different timescales (32, 33). To investigate the dynamic nature of CDI and CSI metrics during PVT, for each metric, we computed systematically 6 time series from each fMRI dataset by using 2 different window lengths (3 and 4 min) in combination with 3 different levels of overlapping (0.50 and 75%).

The means of the time series for CDI and CSI datasets for the participants underwent a paired t-test to assess the FC change pre- vs post-APT-II intervention. The statistical significance was evaluated by a voxel-wise threshold p < 0.001 (t > 4.2) to form initial cluster candidates, followed by a cluster-size limit of n ≥ 54 to warrant a family-wise error rate (FWER) p ≤ 0.01 as determined by permutation simulations. The number of permutations was set to 5 × 104. As discussed in previous studies (34, 35), a QDA framework can derive the connectivity metrics for each local voxel with the rest of the other voxels in the brain and can be used to assess overall how well the local voxel is connected with the rest of brain. As the QDA metrics do not provide information on where and how well specific brain regions are connected, a seed-based analysis using the AAL3 atlas (36) was performed. Using the average time courses of the preprocessed fMRI signals for the AAL3 Region of Interest (ROI), the Pearson correlation coefficients (CC) between different pairs of ROIs defined in AAL3 were computed to form a symmetric cross correlation matrix. The ROI-based analysis of FC is typically measured using static metrics, which uses the entire time series for more accurate and stable estimates of long-term FC.

Case presentations

Case 1. A 20-year-old male (left-handed, 11.5 years of education) suffered a TBI after a fall accident, Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) = 5 (37, 38). Early MRI showed diffuse axonal injury (DAI) of grade III with a subdural haematoma on the left, contusion bleeding in the left frontal lobe and minor bleeding in the internal capsule. Two weeks post-injury at the Intensive Care Unit, he followed an extensive rehabilitation programme (6 weeks inpatient, 5 months home-based, and 4.5 months outpatient rehabilitation). He then resumed studies with an adjusted education curriculum while being admitted to the outpatient brain injury rehabilitation programme for young adults. He reported retrograde amnesia for 2 years before the incident, flat affect, difficulty finding words, difficulty concentrating during reading, and social isolation. He presented an average level of abstract thinking (Matrices, WAIS-IV: raw score [RS] = 19 points, SS = 11 points). The APT test indicated mild attention deficit (sustained attention: 67% and selective attention: 63%). APT-II was administered 18 months post-injury at the age of 21 years and 9 months. When discharged from the rehabilitation team at the age of 25, GOSE was 6 (23).

Case 2. A 21-year-old female (right-handed, 12 years of education) sustained a TBI in a car accident. Medical records reported a short post traumatic amnesia (˂ 24 h) and only a few seconds of loss of consciousness (˂ 30 min), corresponding to GCS = 15. MRI showed a minor tissue loss in the left fronto-basal anterior area, nonspecific supratentorial bilateral white matter changes, and minor extra-axial bleeding residuals on the left side of the surgery area. When medically stable (2 weeks post injury), she received additional inpatient care for 4 weeks. After a planned skull reconstruction, she was referred to home-based rehabilitation (4 weeks). Four months post-injury, she started an adjusted education programme at the university while being admitted to the outpatient brain injury rehabilitation programme for young adults. She reported reduced social interaction, concentration difficulties, distractibility, mnemonic problems, difficulties in word-finding and theory of mind, headache, fatigue, tinnitus, and sensitivity to stress. She attained above average on abstract thinking (Matrices, WAIS-IV: RS = 22 points, SS = 13 points). The APT test indicated moderate to severe attention deficit (complex sustained attention: 50%, selective attention: 40% and alternating attention: 37.5%). APT-II was administered 13 months post-injury, at the age of 22 years and 7 months. When discharged from the rehabilitation team at the age of 25, GOSE was 8.

RESULTS

FC during the psychomotor vigilance test

As shown in Fig. 1 and Table I, differences in the CDI metrics after the APT-II were significant (FWER, p ≤ 0.01) for both participants, with reduced variability post-intervention. The CDI was significantly enhanced in the right frontal eye field (R-FEF), and middle R-superior frontal gyri, R-cuneus and superior occipital gyrus, and bilateral motor-sensory cortex. In contrast, a significant reduction was detected in the following brain regions: thalamus, fusiform gyrus, para-hippocampal gyrus (PHC), cerebellum, middle and posterior cingulate (MCC, PCC), R-supramarginal gyrus (SMG), R-superior temporal gyrus (STG). The CSI metrics showed a similar trend.

Table I. Differences in the connectivity density index metrics post-APT-II intervention between specific brain regions, with results for case 1 and 2, including the cluster size, centre of mass coordinates in MNI-template space (CMx, CMY, and CMz), mean t-score ± the standard error of the mean (SEM) for the ROIs, and their anatomical overlaps with the brain atlas CA_ML_18_MNI
Nr Volume (ml) CMx (mm) CMy (mm) CMz (mm) t-score (± SEM) R/L Overlap with brain atlas template (CA_ML_18_MNI)
1 11,021 −7.1 389 −7.5 −4.37 ± 0.15 B 50.4% Thalamus, 12.3% Fusiform gyrus, 5.3% Parahippocampal Gyrus
2 6993 −9.3 60.7 −45.9 −4.39 ± 0.19 B 41.2% Fusiform gyrus, 30.6% Cerebellum, 11.1% Cerebellar Vermis
3 4644 −55.8 32.9 23.8 −4.42 ± 0.22 R 50% Supramarginal Gyrus, 42.2% R-Superior Temporal Gyrus
4 2835 −1.1 32.4 35.7 −4.38 ± 0.25 B 74.8% MCC, 19.8% PCC,
5 2349 −37.6 13.3 61.3 4.41 ± 0.28 R 80.3% Precentral Gyrus, 19.6% Superior and Middle Frontal Gyri
6 2160 46.0 18.8 52.2 4.42 ± 0.27 L 78.0% Postcentral Gyrus, 20.2% Precentral Gyrus
7 1809 −13.7 88.8 27.6 4.44 ± 0.33 R 59.1% Cuneus, 33.8% Superior Occipital Gyrus
R: right, L: left, B: Bilateral. The ROIs are numbered in descending order of the cluster size.

Figure 1
Fig. 1. Summary of task-state functional connectivity (FC). (A) The average connectivity density index (CDI) metrics for case 1 and 2 pre- vs post-APT-II for the ROIs listed in Table I. The main anatomical areas for ROIs 1–7 are thalamus, fusiform gyrus, R-supramarginal gyrus, MCC, R-precentral gyrus, L-postcentral gyrus, and R-Cuneus, respectively, (B) The paired t-test results for the CDI data pre- vs post-APT II. The colour bar indicates the t-score scales, and the numerical annotations indicate the locations of the ROIs shown in figure 1a and Table 1, (C) Pearson CC matrix differences based on the entire fMRI time course of 20 min long PVT (post-APT – pre-APT-II) for AAL3 ROIs in case 2, (D) Pearson CC for SMG-L and SMG-R with the rest of the AAL3 ROIs in case 2. (E) Pearson CC matrix differences based on the entire fMRI time course of 20 min long PVT (post-APT – pre-APT-II) for AAL3 ROIs in case 1. (F) Pearson CC for SMG-L and SMG-R with the rest of the AAL3 ROIs in case 1.

The Pearson CC matrix indicated somewhat reduced connectivity among the brain regions in the frontal lobe (AAL3 ROI indexed 1–34) for case 2 (Fig. 1C). In contrast, the connectivity with posterior brain regions was enhanced post-APT-II. Similarly, the occipital gyri (ROI index 49–54) exhibited reduced connectivity with the frontal lobe but enhanced connectivity with other visual areas and the rest of the brain. Please note, the region-specific connectivity in parietal, SMG, and angular gyri (ROI indexed 63–70) exhibited different change patterns for the left and right hemispheres (Fig. 1D and F).

The measured reaction time in PVT during fMRI showed mixed results post-APT-II (see Table II). Decreased reaction time was accompanied by relatively high error rates.

Table II. Summary of PVT data for the subjects pre- and post-APT-II. The results are provided in raw scale unless specified. Available reference values for healthy controls are provided
Assessment Case1 Case 2 Control reference
Pre Post Pre Post Male Female
PVTa RT, ms 305 (105) 320 (131) 250 (262) 113 (40) 418 (57) 406 (65)
PVT Error, % 8.2 6.0 12.1 14.5 5.1 4.0
Time for MRIe 18 19 13 14
aPsychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT)(24, 25) was used for assessment of sustained attention during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI); scoring was based on reaction-time (RT) and error-rate (Error). Intervals between stimuli range from 2 to 10 s, with the entire 20-min task administered in a pseudo-random fashion. eThe time for MRI is given in months after the traumatic incident. The reaction time for case 2 is very short compared even to results for healthy volunteers, which are typically above 400 ms (24, 25). This result could be due to an artefact or reduced inhibition often connected to TBI. The error rate in PVT increased in the same participant, probably due to the reduced reaction time. In previous studies, a high error rate is linked to reduced alertness, slower problem-solving, and poorer psychomotor skills (3941). “*” denotes items with statistically significant differences in performance pre- vs post-intervention (p < 0.05 uncorrected for multiple comparison).

DISCUSSION

A local connectivity density increase was detected in the right hemisphere in middle R-superior frontal gyri, R-cuneus and superior occipital gyrus, and also in the FEF, which is a vital part of the dorsal frontoparietal attention system that engages in an orienting process to prioritize visual input (7).

A local connectivity density decrease was found in thalamus, fusiform gyrus, PHC, MCC, PCC, SMG, R-superior temporal gyrus (STG). PCC plays a pivotal role in regulating the focus of attention (42) and is reported to be a part of the default mode network (DMN), the task negative network (3, 4).

The FC variability was reduced for both participants. Prior work (43) has shown reduced variability in psychometric tests, the Paced Auditory Serial Attention Test (PASAT) (44) after APT-II. Our findings provide cautious insight in the recovery process post-training.

The separate change patterns for the left and right hemispheres in parietal, SMG, and angular gyri may support findings saying that right hemisphere is connected to executive attention systems (7). The current results, that is, changed connectivity and reduced variability during PVT, are in line with changes in FC found after attention training (22) and are substantiated by the theory of balance between integrated and segregated networks for a healthy functioning brain (45).

This exploratory pilot study is limited by the small sample size. The connectivity changes might thus be the result of the natural course in the brain after a traumatic lesion since the brain is continuously changing, also in the chronic phase. Nevertheless, the changes in connectivity observed provide a result that justifies further investigations into how the relationship between connectivity and response to attention training can be captured during a psychomotor vigilance test by fMRI. For clinical translation, it is recommended to disclose psychometric data as a supplement to the neuroimaging protocol.

In conclusion, the results showed both an increase and a decrease in connectivity density in frontal, posterior and subcortical brain regions, for some regions with separate change patterns for left and right hemisphere respectively, and an overall reduction in variability in FC (indicating a more robust connectivity) after intensive APT-II in 2 patients with TBI. Whether these FC findings are related to intervention effects cannot be concluded due to a small sample. In future studies, the neuroimaging protocol would benefit from disclosing psychometric data on sustained attention and inhibition.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This case study is part of a larger study, “Effects of systematic attention training after acquired brain injury-structural and functional changes.” The Regional Ethical Committee approved the study; the registration number was 2017/1810-31.

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors without undue reservation if the participants’ privacy is guaranteed.

The local Regional Ethical Committee approved the study; registration number was 2017/1810-31. Written informed consent was obtained from participants to publish any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

This research was supported by a grant from the ALF foundation in Stockholm Region and the Joint China-Sweden Mobility programme from STINT (Dnr: CH2019-8397).

References

  1. Markovic G, Schult ML, Elg M, Bartfai A. Beneficial effects of early attention process training after acquired brain injury: a randomized controlled trial. J Rehabil Med 2019; 52: jrm00011. https://doi.org/10.2340/16501977-2628
  2. Blank IA, Kiran S, Fedorenko E. Can neuroimaging help aphasia researchers? Addressing generalizability, variability, and interpretability. Cogn Neuropsychol 2017; 34: 377–393. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2017.1402756
  3. Chand GB, Dhamala M. Interactions among the brain default-mode, salience, and central-executive networks during perceptual decision-making of moving dots. Brain Connect 2016; 6: 249–254. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2015.0379
  4. Goulden N, Khusnulina A, Davis NJ, Bracewell RM, Bokde AL, McNulty JP, et al. The salience network is responsible for switching between the default mode network and the central executive network: replication from DCM. NeuroImage 2014; 99: 180–190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.052
  5. Li D, Christ SE, Cowan N. Domain-general and domain-specific functional networks in working memory. NeuroImage 2014; 102: 646–656. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.028
  6. Nyberg L. Kognitiv neurovetenskap: studier av sambandet mellan hjärnaktivitet och mentala processer. Lund: Studentlitteratur; 2009.
  7. Petersen SE, Posner MI. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Ann Rev Neurosci 2012; 35: 73–89. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525
  8. Rosenberg MD, Hsu WT, Scheinost D, Todd Constable R, Chun MM. Connectome-based models predict separable components of attention in novel individuals. J Cogn Neurosci 2018; 30: 160–173. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01197
  9. Kim YH, Yoo WK, Ko MH, Park CH, Kim ST, Na DL. Plasticity of the attentional network after brain injury and cognitive rehabilitation. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2009; 23: 468–477. https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968308328728
  10. Sohlberg MM, Mateer CA. Cognitive rehabilitation: an integrative neuropsychological approach. Guilford Press. 2001.
  11. Sohlberg MM, Johnson L, Paule L, Raskin SA, Mateer CA. Attention process training-II: a program to address attentional deficits for persons with mild cognitive dysfunction. Wake Forest, NC: Lash & Associates; 1989.
  12. Sohlberg MM, Mateer CA. Effectiveness of an attention-training program, J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1987; 9: 117–130. DOI: 10.1080/01688638708405352
  13. Sohlberg MM, Mateer CA. Effectiveness of an attention-training program. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1987; 9: 117–130. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638708405352
  14. Rohling ML, Faust ME, Beverly B, Demakis G. Effectiveness of cognitive rehabilitation following acquired brain injury: a meta-analytic re-examination of Cicerone et al.’s (2000, 2005) systematic reviews. Neuropsychology 2009; 23: 20–39. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013659
  15. Eberlee R, Shapiro-Rosenbaum A, editors. The ACRM cognitive rehabilitation manual & textbook-translating evidence based recommendations into practice. 2022. Reston: ACRM Publishing; 2022.
  16. Cicerone KD, Goldin Y, Ganci K, Rosenbaum A, Wethe JV, Langenbahn DM, et al. Evidence-based vognitive rehabilitation: systematic review of the literature from 2009 through 2014. Arch Phys Med Rehabili 2019; 100: 1515–1533. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2019.02.011
  17. Barker-Collo SL, Feigin VL, Lawes CM, Parag V, Senior H, Rodgers A. Reducing attention deficits after stroke using attention process training: a randomized controlled trial. Stroke 2009; 40: 3293–3298. https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.558239
  18. Sohlberg MM, McLaughlin KA, Pavese A, Heidrich A, Posner MI. Evaluation of attention process training and brain injury education in persons with acquired brain injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2000; 22: 656–676. https://doi.org/10.1076/1380-3395(200010)22:5;1-9;FT656
  19. Tiersky LA, Anselmi V, Johnston MV, Kurtyka J, Roosen E, Schwartz T, et al. A trial of neuropsychologic rehabilitation in mild-spectrum traumatic brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2005; 86: 1565–1574. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2005.03.013
  20. Bartfai A, Markovic G, Sargenius Landahl K, Schult ML. The protocol and design of a randomised controlled study on training of attention within the first year after acquired brain injury. BMC Neurol 2014; 14: 102. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2377-14-102
  21. Pantoni L, Poggesi A, Diciotti S, Valenti R, Orsolini S, Della Rocca E, et al. Effect of attention training in mild cognitive impairment patients with subcortical vascular changes: The RehAtt study. J Alzheimer’s Dis 2017; 60: 615–624. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-170428
  22. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D. Neuropsychological assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012.
  23. Wilson L, Boase K, Nelson LD, Temkin NR, Giacino JT, Markowitz AJ, et al. A manual for the Glasgow Outcome Scale-Extended interview. J Neurotrauma 2021; 38: 2435–2446. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2020.7527
  24. Moller MC, Nordin LE, Bartfai A, Julin P, Li TQ. Fatigue and cognitive fatigability in mild traumatic brain injury are correlated with altered neural activity during vigilance test performance. Front Neurol 2017; 8: 496. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2017.00496
  25. Nordin LE, Moller MC, Julin P, Bartfai A, Hashim F, Li TQ. Post mTBI fatigue is associated with abnormal brain functional connectivity. Sci Rep 2016; 6: 21183. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep21183
  26. Thompson BJ, Shugart C, Dennison K, Louder TJ. Test-retest reliability of the 5-minute psychomotor vigilance task in working-aged females. J Neurosci Methods 2022; 365: 109379. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2021.109379
  27. Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med 1995; 34: 537–541. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1910340409
  28. Li TQ, Wang Y, Hallin R, Juto JE. Resting-state fMRI study of acute migraine treatment with kinetic oscillation stimulation in nasal cavity. NeuroImage Clin 2016; 12: 451–459. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2016.08.014
  29. Wang Y, Berglund IS, Uppman M, Li TQ. Juvenile myoclonic epilepsy has hyper dynamic functional connectivity in the dorsolateral frontal cortex. Neuroimage Clin 2019; 21: 101604. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2018.11.014
  30. Li X, Fischer H, Manzouri A, Månsson KNT, Li TQ. A quantitative data-driven analysis framework for resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging: a study of the impact of adult age. Front Neurosci 2021; 15: 768418. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2021.768418
  31. Li X, Fischer H, Manzouri A, Månsson KNT, Li TQ. Dataset of whole-brain resting-state fMRI of 227 young and elderly adults acquired at 3T. Data Brief 2021; 38: 107333. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dib.2021.107333
  32. Hindriks R, Adhikari MH, Murayama Y, Ganzetti M, Mantini D, Logothetis NK, et al. Can sliding-window correlations reveal dynamic functional connectivity in resting-state fMRI? NeuroImage 2016; 127: 242–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.055
  33. Mokhtari F, Akhlaghi MI, Simpson SL, Wu G, Laurienti PJ. Sliding window correlation analysis: Modulating window shape for dynamic brain connectivity in resting state. NeuroImage 2019; 189: 655–666. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.001
  34. Tjernström N, Li TQ, Holst S, Roman E. Functional connectivity in reward-related networks is associated with individual differences in gambling strategies in male Lister hooded rats. Addict Biol 2022; 27: e13131. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.13131
  35. Li X, Fischer H, Manzouri A, Månssonb K, Li TQ. A quantitative data-driven analysis (QDA) framework for resting-state fMRI: a study of the impact of adult age. Front Neurosci 2021; 15: 768418
  36. Rolls ET, Huang CC, Lin CP, Feng J, Joliot M. Automated anatomical labelling atlas 3. Neuroimage 2020; 206: 116189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116189
  37. Bellner J, Jensen SM, Lexell J, Romner B. Diagnostic criteria and the use of ICD-10 codes to define and classify minor head injury. J Neurol NeurosurgPsychiatry 2003; 74: 351–352. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.74.3.351
  38. Teasdale G, Maas A, Lecky F, Manley G, Stocchetti N, Murray G. The Glasgow Coma Scale at 40 years: standing the test of time. Lancet Neurol 2014; 13: 844–854. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70120-6
  39. Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep 2003; 26: 117–126. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/26.2.117
  40. Drummond SP, Bischoff-Grethe A, Dinges DF, Ayalon L, Mednick SC, Meloy MJ. The neural basis of the psychomotor vigilance task. Sleep 2005; 28: 1059–1068.
  41. Lim J, Dinges DF. A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychol Bull 2010; 136: 375–389. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018883
  42. Leech R, Sharp DJ. The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain J Neurol 2014; 137: 12–32. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt162
  43. Markovic G, Schult ML, Bartfai A, Elg M. Statistical process control: a feasibility study of the application of time-series measurement in early neurorehabilitation after acquired brain injury. J Rehabil Med 2017; 49: 128–135. https://doi.org/10.2340/16501977-2172
  44. Gronwall DM. Paced auditory serial-addition task: a measure of recovery from concussion. Percept Motor Skills 1977; 44: 367–373. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1977.44.2.367
  45. Wig GS. Segregated systems of human brain networks. Trends Cogn Sci. 2017; 21: 981–996. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.09.006